Email updates

Keep up to date with the latest news and content from JCR and BioMed Central.

Open Access Highly Accessed Short paper

Time for sex: nycthemeral distribution of human sexual behavior

Roberto Refinetti

Author Affiliations

Circadian Rhythm Laboratory, University of South Carolina, 807 Hampton Street, Walterboro, SC 29488, USA

Journal of Circadian Rhythms 2005, 3:4  doi:10.1186/1740-3391-3-4


The electronic version of this article is the complete one and can be found online at: http://www.jcircadianrhythms.com/content/3/1/4


Received:15 March 2005
Accepted:24 March 2005
Published:24 March 2005

© 2005 Refinetti; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Background

Nycthemeral (daily) oscillation has been documented in a variety of physiological and behavioral processes. The present study was carried out to evaluate the existence of a nycthemeral rhythm of human sexual behavior and to identify environmental factors responsible for the rhythmic pattern.

Methods

Non-traditional university students (ages 18 to 51 years) recorded the times of day when they went to sleep, when they woke up, and when they had sex for 3 consecutive weeks. They also answered a questionnaire designed to identify the causes of their selection of time for sex.

Results

The majority of sexual encounters took place at bedtime (11 pm to 1 am). The most common explanations for this temporal pattern were the rigidity of the professional work schedule and family obligations and the availability of the partner, which reduced the opportunity for sexual encounters at other times of the day.

Conclusion

Most sexual encounters take place around bedtime. Although the presence of an endogenous component responsible for this temporal pattern cannot be excluded, the evidence indicates strong environmental forcing, particularly from the work/family schedule of the individuals and from partner availability.

Background

Practically all physiological parameters in the animal and human body exhibit nycthemeral (daily) or circadian oscillation [1,2]. Epidemiological studies have documented nycthemeral oscillation in a variety of aggregate variables, such as heart attacks [3-5], births [6-8], and suicides [9-11]. One study conducted in 1982 provided evidence of the existence of a nycthemeral rhythm of sexual activity in young married couples [12]. The present study sought to verify this nycthemeral rhythmicity in a sample of human adults with a wider age range and to identify environmental factors responsible for the rhythmic pattern.

Methods

Part 1

The first part of the study, conducted during the winter of 2003, involved 15 non-traditional university students in South Carolina (6 males, 9 females, ages 18 to 51 years). The subjects participated in the study as partial fulfillment of course requirements. They were given a data sheet and were asked to record, each day for three weeks, the times of day when they went to sleep, when they woke up, and when they had sex. Subjects were allowed to use their own definition of sex, which did not necessarily involve vaginal intercourse. Complete forms were received from 11 subjects, anonymously. All subjects were married or were involved in a steady relationship with a partner.

The accumulated nycthemeral distribution of sexual encounters was analyzed in three ways. A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test [13] was used to determine whether the experimental distribution differed significantly from a flat distribution. A Rayleigh test [14] was used to determine whether the experimental distribution conformed to a sinusoidal pattern. A cosinor test [15] was used to determine the acrophase (time of peak) of the nycthemeral oscillation.

Part 2

The second part of the study, conducted during the winter of 2005, involved a separate group of 38 university students (14 males, 24 females, ages 18 to 43 years). The subjects were given a brief survey containing two main questions: "What time of day do you usually have sex?" and "Why do you have sex at these times (as opposed to other times of day)?". Answers could be written in for both questions, but potential answers were provided for the second question, as follows:

__ I feel more sexual at these times.

__ These are the only times when my partner is available.

__ My work/family schedule does not allow me to have sex at other times.

__ I'm already in bed, so why not have sex?

__ Other. Specify: ________________________________

Ten subjects (4 males, 6 females, aged between 18 and 20 years) had never had sex and, therefore, were excluded from the study. The percentages of the answers to the second question were computed and compared by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test.

Results and Discussion

Part 1

Although there were not enough data for the analysis of the distribution of sexual encounters according to day of the week, the mean number of sexual encounters per week (2.3) was comparable to that found in the broader adult population of the United States (twice a week) [16].

The nycthemeral distribution of sexual encounters is shown in Figure 1. Of 71 recorded encounters, 17 took place at midnight, which was the average to-bed time for the subjects. A smaller peak occurred at 06:00, which was the average wake-up time for the subjects. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test indicated that the distribution was significantly different from a flat distribution (D = 0.201, p < 0.01), and the Rayleigh test indicated that the distribution conformed to a sinusoidal pattern (nR2 = 15.706, χ2(2) = 31.412, p < 0.0001). The cosinor test indicated an acrophase at 01:00.

thumbnailFigure 1. Nycthemeral distribution of sexual encounters. Of 71 recorded encounters, 17 took place at midnight, which was the average to-bed time for the subjects.

The mean bedtime of 00:00 and mean waketime of 06:00 are within the range of bedtimes and waketimes observed in various societies around the world [17-23]. The observed bedtime peak of sexual encounters is in agreement with a previous study of young married couples [12]. The results are also in agreement with those obtained in a survey about the time of day of the first intercourse of young women, in which it was found that over 85% of the subjects had lost their virginity either in the evening or at night [24].

Part 2

Because the subjects in Part2 were asked only to recall their usual time of sex, the precision of the results was lower than in Part 1. The answers were categorized as "morning" (20%), "afternoon" (5%), "evening" (10%), and "night" (65%). This distribution is significantly different from a flat distribution (D = 0.400, p < 0.001) and is consistent with the results from Part 1.

The distribution of responses to the question "Why do you have sex at these times?" is shown in Figure 2. Although the subjects were allowed to give multiple answers, most of them gave only one answer, and the few who chose "Other" actually wrote in one the four offered options. Consequently, the percentages of answers conveniently added up to 100%. Only 28% of the respondents gave an answer that would characterize an endogenous rhythm of sexual appetite ("I feel more sexual at these times"). In contrast, 33% of the respondents attributed the time selection to restrictions imposed by work/family schedule, and 23% attributed it to partner availability. The distribution of percentages does not differ significantly from a flat distribution (D = 0.090, p > 0.05), which indicates that none of the four responses was consistently chosen more frequently than the others. Thus, the response based on an endogenous rhythm of sexual appetite was chosen by only a quarter of the respondents. Even this relatively small fraction may be an overestimate, as the subjects may have failed to identify environmental factors other than those offered as potential answers.

thumbnailFigure 2. Frequency distribution of answers to the question "Why do you have sex at these times?" Most subjects indicated that the selection of time for sex is determined by external factors, such as work schedule and partner availability.

In addressing the issue of exogenous causation, it must be emphasized that this study was conducted under normal living conditions, where environmental factors can "mask" the expression of an endogenous rhythm of sexual appetite. In men, it is known that there is daily rhythmicity in plasma concentration of testosterone [25,26], which likely has an endogenous source and could be responsible for rhythmicity in sexual appetite. However, the results of this study clearly indicate that, if there is an endogenous rhythm of sexual appetite, it is not the main determinant of the selection of time for sex in individuals living under normal societal conditions. Instead, the time for sex seems to be determined predominantly by environmental forcing.

Conclusion

Although human adults seem to find opportunities for sex at practically any time of the day, most sexual encounters occur around bedtime (11 pm to 1 am). A smaller peak in sexual activity occurs around wake time. Because the study was conducted in the presence of external time cues, the issue of the endogenous source of this nycthemeral variation cannot be directly addressed. However, self-reports indicate the presence of strong environmental forcing, particularly from the work/family schedule of the individuals and from partner availability.

Competing Interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

References

  1. Dunlap JC, Loros JJ, DeCoursey PJ: Chronobiology: Biological Timekeeping. Sunderland, Mass: Sinauer; 2004. OpenURL

  2. Refinetti R: Circadian Physiology. 2nd edition. Boca Raton, Fla: CRC Press; 2005. OpenURL

  3. Fromm RE, Levine RL, Pepe PE: Circadian variation in the time of request for helicopter transport of cardiac patients.

    Ann Emerg Med 1992, 21:1196-1199. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  4. Spielberg C, Falkenhahn D, Willich SN, Wegscheider K, Völler H: Circadian, day-of-week, and seasonal variability in myocardial infarction: comparison between working and retired patients.

    Am Heart J 1996, 132:579-585. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  5. López-Messa JB, Garmendia-Leiza JR, Aguilar-García MD, Andrés-de-Llano JM, Alberola-López C, Ardura-Fernández J: Factores de riesgo cardiovascular en el ritmo circadiano del infarto agudo de miocardio.

    Rev Esp Cardiol 2004, 57:850-858. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  6. Fraser WD, McLean FH, Usher RH: Diurnal variation in admission to hospital of women in labour.

    Can J Surg 1989, 32:33-35. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  7. Anderka M, Declercq ER, Smith W: A time to be born.

    Am J Public Health 2000, 90:124-126. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  8. Goldstick O, Weissman A, Drugan A: The circadian rhythm of "urgent" operative deliveries.

    Isr Med Assoc J 2003, 5:564-566. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  9. Gallerani M, Avato FM, Dal Monte D, Caracciolo S, Fersini C, Manfredini R: The time for suicide.

    Psychol Med 1996, 26:867-870. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  10. Preti A, Miotto P: Diurnal variations in suicide by age and gender in Italy.

    J Affect Disord 2001, 65:253-261. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  11. Van Houwelingen CAJ, Beersma DGM: Seasonal changes in 24-h patterns of suicide rates: a study on train suicides in The Netherlands.

    J Affect Disord 2001, 66:215-223. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  12. Palmer JD, Udry JR, Morris NM: Diurnal and weekly, but no lunar rhythm in human copulation.

    Hum Biol 1982, 54:111-121. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  13. Krauth J: Distribution-Free Statistics: An Application-Oriented Approach. New York: Elsevier; 1988. OpenURL

  14. Mardia KV: Statistics of Directional Data. New York: Academic Press; 1972. OpenURL

  15. Nelson W, Tong YL, Lee JK, Halberg F: Methods for cosinor rhythmometry.

    Chronobiologia 1979, 6:305-323. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  16. Laumann EO, Gagnon JH, Michael RT, Michaels S: The Social Organization of Sexuality: Sexual Practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1994. OpenURL

  17. Binkley S, Tome MB, Crawford D, Mosher K: Human daily rhythms measured for one year.

    Physiol Behav 1990, 48:293-298. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  18. Refinetti R: Persistence of synchronization of the daily rhythms of body temperature and sleep-wake in college students.

    Biol Rhythm Res 1995, 26:532-540. OpenURL

  19. Park YM, Matsumoto K, Seo YJ, Shinkoda H: Scores on morningness-eveningness and sleep habits of Korean students, Japanese students, and Japanese workers.

    Percept Mot Skills 1997, 85:143-154. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  20. Laberge L, Carrier J, Lespérance P, Lambert C, Vitaro F, Tremblay RE, Montplaisir J: Sleep and circadian phase characteristics of adolescent and young adult males in a naturalistic summertime condition.

    Chronobiol Int 2000, 17:489-501. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  21. Medeiros ALD, Mendes DBF, Lima PF, Araujo JF: The relationships between sleep-wake cycle and academic performance in medical students.

    Biol Rhythm Res 2001, 32:263-270. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  22. Martin SK, Eastman CI: Sleep logs of young adults with self-selected sleep times predict the dim light melatonin onset.

    Chronobiol Int 2002, 19:695-707. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  23. Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M: Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes.

    J Biol Rhythms 2003, 18:80-90. PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  24. Barak Y, Stein D, Ring A, Ticher A, Elizur A: Patterns of first intercourse: a survey among Israeli women.

    Biol Rhythm Res 1997, 28:36-41. Publisher Full Text OpenURL

  25. Reinberg A, Lagoguey M, Cesselin F, Touitou Y, Legrand JC, Delassalle A, Antreassian J, Lagoguey M: Circadian and circannual rhythms in plasma hormones and other variables of five healthy young human males.

    Acta Endocrinol 1978, 88:417-427. PubMed Abstract OpenURL

  26. Marrama P, Carani C, Baraghini GF, Volpe A, Zini D, Celani MF, Montanini V: Circadian rhythm of testosterone and prolactin in the ageing.

    Maturitas 1982, 4:131-138. PubMed Abstract OpenURL